Leibniz-Zentrum Allgemeine Sprachwissenschaft Leibniz-Gemeinschaft

When humans gesture, they activate an ancient connection between movement and breathing

02.09.2022

Press release September 2nd, 2022

When humans gesture, they activate an ancient connection between movement and breathing

Researchers from Radboud University and ZAS Berlin have developed a new perspective on why humans often move their hands rhythmically during speaking. The perspective published in Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, suggests that there are striking connections between animal and human behavior when it comes to moving with sounds. For example, flying bats synchronize their echo-locating vocalization and their wingbeats which can be directly related to how humans synchronize acoustic features of their voice and move their upper limbs.

“When a bat echo-locates while flying, the muscle tensioning that drive the wingbeats also leads to small compressions of the lungs. It becomes more economical for the bat to vocalize during these particular moments of the wingbeat cycle, leading to synchronization,” says researcher Wim Pouw at the Donders Center for Brain, Cognition, and Behaviour. Pouw and his collaborator, Susanne Fuchs from Leibniz-Centre General Linguistics (ZAS) suggest that this is comparable to what happens when humans produce rhythmic hand gestures, as another line of research has shown that these hand gestures affect rib cage movements, which slightly change the volume of the lungs, and thereby change aspects of the voice.

“Once we started reading up on very different lines of research, we were astonished how prevalent it is for animals to time their vocalizations together with movements. We discovered that coupling movement and vocalization might be very important for the development of vocal repertoires,” Dr. Fuchs says. In a particular kind of bird, for example, flightless chicklets have a very typical vocalization. But the vocalization quality drastically changes when the chicklets start using their wings in the nest to practice flying. Vocalization becomes similar to adult vocalizations when they start using their wings for flight. “This is exactly what is found in human infants around 9 months of age!” Pouw says, “when infants start to rhythmically “babble” with their hands in an exploratory fashion, they also start to “babble” in speech in the form of reduplicated syllables such as mama or papa”. Rhythmic movements may thus have very early connections with the voice, which explains why movement and sounds are so fundamentally related.

This new perspective of how gesture relates to the voice via interactions with respiration provides an answer to why humans started to move their hands while vocalizing and speaking: These systems physically interacted throughout evolution, and this development is in a way mirrored by infants who start to learn to move and vocalize at the same time. “Many researchers of communication believe that manual gestures were for showing things,” Pouw says “but we actually think this not the whole story… in a way gestures are also part of what it means for humans to speak, which is not a passive affair, but an orchestra of movement to generate meaning”.


The Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour at Radboud University is a world-class research centre devoted to understanding the mechanistic underpinnings of human cognition and behavior in health and disease. The Institute is home to more than 600 researchers from 35 countries who share the common goal of contributing to the advancement of the brain-, cognitive- and behavioral sciences through investigator-driven research, and improving health, education and technology by applying advances in this field.
The Leibniz-Zentrum Allgemeine Sprachwissenschaft (ZAS) is a university-independent research institute located in the federal state of Berlin. Its objective is the investigation of natural language and its manifestation in individual languages. The aim is to better understand this central human ability and its biological, cognitive and social factors.


Contact for scientific information:

Dr. Wim Pouw: wim.pouw@donders.ru.nl, Tel: 0031630030071
Dr. Susanne Fuchs: fuchs@leibniz-zas.de, Tel: 004917646184175


Original publication:

Pouw, W. & Fuchs, S. "Origins of vocal-entangled gesture." In: Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2022.104836